Connect with us
We’re experimenting with AI-generated content to help deliver information faster and more efficiently.
While we try to keep things accurate, this content is part of an ongoing experiment and may not always be reliable.
Please double-check important details — we’re not responsible for how the information is used.

Behavioral Science

Echidna Pseudo-Pouch Microbiome Shifts During Lactation Helps Young Thrive

Research shows microbial communities in echidna pseudo-pouches undergo dramatic changes while the animal is lactating, which could help in creating an environment for their young, known as puggles, to thrive.

Avatar photo

Published

on

Echidnas, the only mammals that lay eggs, have an unusual reproductive system that includes a pseudo-pouch where their young, called puggles, grow and develop during lactation. Researchers from the University of Adelaide have made a fascinating discovery about the microbiome in these pseudo-pouches, which changes significantly while the mother is nursing her young.

The study, published in FEMS Microbiology Ecology, reveals that the microbial communities in echidna pseudo-pouches undergo dramatic changes during lactation, creating an environment that’s conducive to the health and well-being of their puggles. This is particularly important since puggles hatch at a very early developmental stage, lacking a functional immune system.

“We know that the reproductive microbiome is crucial for infant health in many species, including humans,” says Isabella Wilson, lead researcher on the study. “However, little was known about how it functions in egg-laying monotremes like echidnas.”

One of the key findings of this research is that during lactation, the pseudo-pouch microbial communities show significant differences in composition compared to samples taken outside of breeding season or during courtship and mating. This suggests that the echidna pseudo-pouch environment changes during lactation to accommodate young that lack a functional adaptive immune system.

The way puggles suckle may contribute to this shift in microbes. Unlike other species, echidnas don’t have nipples; instead, their young rub their beaks against a part of the pseudo-pouch called the milk patch, causing milk to come out of the skin, similar to a sweat or oil gland.

Compounds within the milk and from the skin probably contribute to the changes seen in the pseudo-pouch microbiota during lactation. This study highlights the importance of understanding these unique reproductive dynamics for conservation efforts and breeding programs for echidnas.

The research also sheds light on previous findings that showed big differences in the gut microbiome between echidnas in zoos and those in the wild. Surprisingly, no major difference was found in the pseudo-pouch microbiota between zoo-managed and wild animals. This suggests that the milk, rather than external environmental factors like captivity, is what primarily shapes the bacterial landscape of the pseudo-pouch.

For conservation efforts and breeding programs, it’s essential to learn more about the bacteria found in echidna pseudo-pouches and how they affect echidna health. This knowledge will help ensure the well-being of these unique animals and their young, ultimately contributing to the preservation of this fascinating species.

Agriculture and Food

Can Plants Hear Their Pollinators?

When pollinators visit flowers, they produce various sounds, from wing flapping during hovering, to landing and takeoff. Scientists studied these vibroacoustic signals to develop noninvasive and efficient methods for monitoring pollinator communities and their influences on plant biology and ecology. The researchers found that the bee sounds led the snapdragons to increase their sugar and nectar volume, and even alter their gene expression that governs sugar transport and nectar production.

Avatar photo

Published

on

Can plants hear their pollinators? While this question may seem like a far-fetched concept, researchers have discovered that plants can indeed detect the buzzing sounds produced by insects as they visit flowers. This groundbreaking finding has significant implications for our understanding of plant-pollinator coevolution and could potentially lead to new methods for improving crop yields.

Professor Francesca Barbero from the University of Turin and her team of collaborators have been studying the acoustic signals produced by pollinators, such as bees and butterflies, as they interact with flowers. They played recordings of these sounds near growing snapdragons and found that the plants responded to the vibroacoustic cues by increasing their sugar and nectar volume. In some cases, the plants even altered their gene expression in response to the signals.

This discovery has shed new light on the complex relationships between plants and their pollinators. By detecting the distinctive sounds produced by efficient pollinators, plants may be able to adapt their behavior to improve their reproductive success. For example, a plant may respond to the sound of a bee by increasing its nectar production or altering its gene expression to attract more pollinators.

The team is now conducting further research to explore the potential applications of this discovery. They are analyzing how plants respond to different pollinators and nectar robbers, and they hope to develop new methods for improving crop yields using sound-based technologies.

As Barbero notes, “The ability to discriminate approaching pollinators based on their distinctive vibroacoustic signals could be an adaptive strategy for plants.” This innovative research has the potential to revolutionize our understanding of plant-pollinator interactions and may lead to significant breakthroughs in agriculture.

Continue Reading

Behavioral Science

“The Scent of Death: Worms Detect Dead Counterparts through a Universal Signal”

Research reveals that for C. elegans worms, the presence of dead members of their species has profound behavioral and physiological effects, leading them to more quickly reproduce and shortening their lifespans.

Avatar photo

Published

on

Here is the rewritten article:

The presence of dead individuals has profound effects across species. From insects like bees and ants removing dead members from their hive to keep the nest clean, to the aversion humans have for decaying bodies, it’s clear that death elicits strong responses. Research at the University of Michigan has shed new light on this phenomenon in the roundworm C. elegans.

In a study published in Cell Reports, researchers discovered that when exposed to dead counterparts, C. elegans experience altered fertility and lifespan. The presence of decedents led to quick reproduction and shortened lifespans in the worms. This response is remarkable considering the worms’ inability to see.

“We were fascinated by this unique opportunity to explore what drives the reaction of C. elegans to a dead conscript,” said Matthias Truttmann, Ph.D., senior author on the paper. His lab studies protein function and aging, making C. elegans an ideal model for studying life extension due to their relatively short lifespans.

The researchers observed that when worms were placed near deceased counterparts, they would move as far away as possible. They hypothesized that there might be a universal death signal emitted by corpses. To test this, they introduced either worm corpses or fluid from the broken-down cells of worm corpses to different feeding areas on a plate. The results showed strong avoidance behavior in C. elegans for both.

Furthermore, the team found that death perception led to reduced fitness in exposed worms and a short-term increase in egg laying. They then systematically tested the worms’ sensory neurons to determine which were necessary for the perception of death. Two olfactory information-responding neurons, AWB and ASH, were identified as key players.

The researchers discovered two metabolites, AMP and histidine, which are normally found inside cells, serve as the death cues for C. elegans. These intracellular metabolites indicate that a cell has died or broken open, signaling something has gone wrong. The presence of these metabolites could very well be an evolutionarily maintained signal of death.

Truttmann points to recent findings in humans where cells undergoing apoptosis release metabolites leading to transcriptional changes in neighboring tissue. Further research is needed to understand how the detection of this signal translates into altered health and behavior.

Continue Reading

Animal Learning and Intelligence

Mice Use Chemical Cues to Sense Social Hierarchy: A Fascinating Phenomenon Shared with Humans

Researchers have shown that mice use chemical cues, including odors, to detect the social rank of an unfamiliar mouse and compare it to their own, using this information to determine their behavior.

Avatar photo

Published

on

The researchers at the Francis Crick Institute have made a groundbreaking discovery: mice use chemical cues, including odors, to sense the social hierarchy of unfamiliar mice and compare it to their own. This remarkable phenomenon has been shown to influence the behavior of mice in confrontations with other mice.

Unlike previous suggestions that mice may display fixed behavior regardless of who they interact with, or that physical properties can give cues about social ranking, the new research published in Current Biology reveals that mice instead infer an unfamiliar mouse’s rank through chemical cues transmitted in the air (odors) or through direct contact (non-volatile scent cues).

The Crick team conducted a series of experiments to demonstrate this remarkable ability. They created a test where male mice entered a transparent tube at opposite ends, meeting in the middle. In this type of confrontation, a more submissive animal will typically retreat. The researchers first observed interactions between mice who shared the same cage, ranking each mouse on a hierarchy before observing how the mice responded to a set of unfamiliar opponents.

The results showed that the strangers could recognize each other’s rank, compare it to their own, and either retreat or force the other mouse to retreat. To further investigate this phenomenon, the team tested the mice in the dark, finding that this did not affect rank recognition, suggesting that cues like physical size or behavior don’t determine recognition of a more aggressive opponent.

The researchers then experimentally blocked the two chemosensory systems that mice use – one for odors in the air (olfactory system) and one for chemical signals transmitted by physical contact (vomeronasal system). They found no effect when just one of these systems was removed; both needed to be ablated before the mice couldn’t recognize opponent rank. This showed that mice use both olfactory and vomeronasal systems to recognize rank and can compensate if one is missing.

Like humans, mice are able to infer the social status of others around them relative to their own, using sensory cues such as language, facial expression, or clothing. The next step for the researchers is to investigate which areas of the brain process the information on opponent rank and own rank and initiate a decision to retreat or advance.

This remarkable phenomenon offers an interesting perspective on social mobility: humans, like mice, can enter a new group of people but still maintain understanding of our own social rank and gauge the social status of unfamiliar people. The State-Dependent Neural Processing Laboratory studies how processes within the brain are impacted by the state of the body, with the hope of advancing a more integrative view of brain physiology in health and disease.

Continue Reading

Trending