Connect with us
We’re experimenting with AI-generated content to help deliver information faster and more efficiently.
While we try to keep things accurate, this content is part of an ongoing experiment and may not always be reliable.
Please double-check important details — we’re not responsible for how the information is used.

Behavioral Science

Unlocking the Secrets of Synchronization Phenomena: Groundbreaking Study Reveals New Complexities

A researcher has conducted an experimental demonstration of intricate and previously theorized behaviors in the fundamental patterns that govern oscillatory systems in nature and technology.

Avatar photo

Published

on

The scientific community has made a significant breakthrough in understanding the intricate patterns that govern oscillatory systems in nature and technology. Researchers from Aston University have conducted the first experimental demonstration of complex behaviors in synchronization regions, also known as Arnold’s tongues.

These synchronization regions are observed in various natural phenomena, such as heartbeats, pendulum swings, or flashing lights. Theoretical studies have suggested that under strong forcing, these regions could take on unexpected shapes, including leaf-like patterns and gaps representing unsynchronized states. Until now, confirming such predictions experimentally had remained a significant challenge.

The new study, led by Dr. Sonia Boscolo from Aston Institute of Photonic Technologies in collaboration with scientists from East China Normal University and the University of Burgundy in France, has been published in Science Advances. The researchers used a breathing-soliton laser to observe these predicted behaviors, confirming the existence of leaf-like structures and ray-like patterns.

The breakthrough builds on previous studies by Dr. Boscolo and her collaborators, which established breathing-soliton lasers as an excellent platform for exploring complex synchronization and chaotic dynamics. Unlike traditional systems that rely on external influences or coupled oscillators, these lasers provide a self-contained environment to study these behaviors.

Dr. Boscolo stated, “This discovery represents a major leap forward in our understanding of nonlinear systems. By experimentally confirming these intricate synchronisation patterns, we open the door for further research into unusual synchronisation phenomena across various physical systems.”

The findings are expected to have broad implications across multiple disciplines, potentially influencing fields such as neuroscience, telecommunications, and space science. The ability to manipulate synchronization regions could lead to new advancements in medical diagnostics, signal processing, and optical communications.

This study is a significant step forward in understanding the complexities of synchronization phenomena and has the potential to revolutionize various fields by providing new insights into nonlinear systems.

Behavioral Science

“A Patchwork of Perspectives: Hong Kong Residents’ Views on Feral Buffalo in their Midst”

Feral water buffalo now roam Hong Kong s South Lantau marshes, and a 657-person survey shows they ignite nostalgia, wonder, and worry in equal measure. Many residents embrace them as living links to a fading rural past and potential conservation icons, yet others fear road blockages, safety hazards, and ecosystem impacts. Attitudes skew more positive among younger locals familiar with wildlife, while concern rises in groups with less rural exposure.

Avatar photo

Published

on

As you venture deeper into this unique landscape, notice the diverse reactions from locals as they interact with their feral neighbors. Some are filled with wonder, while others express concern for safety or environmental impact. This image captures the essence of the complex relationships between humans and wildlife in a semi-urban setting, where natural beauty and urbanization coexist in an intricate dance.

The City University of Hong Kong-led study reveals that public attitudes toward the buffalo fall into four key categories: appreciation and conditional acceptance; concern about community impacts; seeing them as valuable for conservation and education; and individual perceptions formed through everyday encounters. Neutral responses were most frequent, followed by positive and then negative responses.

Regarding the questions on Buffalo Tolerance and Appreciation, 61% of the responses were neutral, 25% highly positive, and 14% highly negative, with effects of age, gender, ethnicity, and birthplace. Looking at the questions on Buffalo Social Benefits and Advocacy, 66% of responses were neutral, 19% highly positive, and 15% highly negative, with significant effects of age and ethnicity.

A similar pattern was found for questions on Preservation and Education, where 46% of the responses were neutral, 41% highly positive, and 13% highly negative, with effects of gender, ethnicity, and birthplace. In the final section on Impacts on Daily Life, 49% of the responses were neutral, 27% highly positive, and 23% highly negative, with significant effects of age and ethnicity.

The study also found that familiarity with wildlife in rural areas often leads to more positive perceptions among participants. This suggests that education and exposure can play a crucial role in shaping attitudes toward feral animals in semi-urban settings.

Ultimately, the research highlights the importance of considering diverse perspectives when managing human-wildlife interactions in shared landscapes. By understanding the complexities of these relationships, we can work towards creating harmonious coexistence between humans and wildlife, even in the most unexpected places.

Continue Reading

Agriculture and Food

Hovering Fish Burn Twice the Energy – Study Shocks Scientists

Hovering fish aren’t loafing—they burn twice resting energy to make micro-fin tweaks that counteract a natural tendency to tip, and body shape dictates just how costly the pause is. The discovery flips a long-held assumption about effortless neutral buoyancy and offers fresh blueprints for agile, instability-embracing underwater robots.

Avatar photo

Published

on

“Fish make hanging motionless in the water column look effortless, and scientists had long assumed that this meant it was a type of rest,” begins the article. However, a new study reveals that fish use nearly twice as much energy when hovering in place compared to resting.

The study, led by scientists at the University of California San Diego’s Scripps Institution of Oceanography, also details the biomechanics of fish hovering, which includes constant, subtle fin movements to prevent tipping, drifting or rolling. This more robust understanding of how fish actively maintain their position could inform the design of underwater robots or drones facing similar challenges.

The findings, published on July 7 in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, overturn the long-standing assumption in the scientific literature that maintaining a stationary position in water is virtually effortless for fish with swim bladders. The reason for this assumption was that nearly all bony fishes have gas-filled sacs called swim bladders that allow them to achieve neutral buoyancy — neither sinking nor rising to the surface. The presence of a swim bladder and the stillness of hovering fish caused the research community to assume hovering was a form of rest that was easy for fish to maintain.

Prior research from lead study author and Scripps marine biologist Valentina Di Santo found that the energy required for skates to swim at various speeds followed a distinct U-shaped curve, with slow and fast swimming requiring the most energy and intermediate speeds being the most energy-efficient. Based on these findings, Di Santo suspected there might be more to hovering than meets the eye.

To learn more, Di Santo and her co-authors conducted experiments with 13 species of fishes with swim bladders. The team placed each fish in a specialized tank and recorded their oxygen consumption during active hovering and motionless resting (when the fish supports its weight with the bottom of the tank). While the fish were hovering, the researchers filmed them with high-speed cameras to capture their fin movements, tracking how each fin moved and how frequently they beat.

The researchers also took a variety of measurements of each fish’s body size and shape. In particular, the scientists measured the physical separation between the fish’s center of mass, which is determined by weight distribution, and its center of buoyancy, which is related to the shape and location of its swim bladder. All these measurements provided a way to quantify how stable or unstable each fish was.

The study found that, contrary to previous assumptions, hovering burns roughly twice as much energy as resting. “Hovering is a bit like trying to balance on a bicycle that’s not moving,” said Di Santo.

Despite having swim bladders that make them nearly weightless, fish are inherently unstable because their center of mass and center of buoyancy don’t align perfectly. This separation creates a tendency to tip and roll, forcing fish to make continuous adjustments with their fins to maintain position. The study found that species with greater separation between their centers of mass and buoyancy used more energy when hovering. This suggests that counteracting instability is one of the factors driving the energy expended during hovering.

“What struck me was how superbly all these fishes maintain a stable posture, despite their intrinsic instability,” said Di Santo.

A fish’s shape and the position of its pectoral fins also influenced its hovering efficiency. Fish with pectoral fins farther back on their body were generally able to burn less energy while hovering, which Di Santo suggested may be due to improved leverage. Long, slender fish, such as the shell dweller cichlid (Lamprologus ocellatus) and the angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare), were found to be less efficient hoverers compared to more compact species like the guppy (Poecilia reticulata) and the zebrafish (Danio rerio).

The study’s findings have significant implications for the design of underwater robots. “By studying how fish achieve this balance, we can gain powerful design principles for building more efficient, responsive underwater technologies,” said Di Santo.

In particular, the findings could help improve the maneuverability of underwater robots, which could allow them to access and explore complex, hard-to-navigate environments like coral reefs or shipwrecks. According to Di Santo, underwater robots have historically been designed with compact shapes that make them stable. As in fish, shapes with more built-in stability are less maneuverable.

“If you want a robot that can maneuver through tight spaces, you might have to learn from these fishes to design in some instability and then add systems that can dynamically maintain stability when needed,” said Di Santo.

The study was co-authored by Xuewei Qi of Stockholm University, Fidji Berio of Scripps Oceanography, Angela Albi of Stockholm University, the Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior, and the University of Konstanz, and Otar Akanyeti of Aberystwyth University in Wales. The research was supported by the Swedish Research Council, the European Commission, the Stockholm University Brain Imaging Centre and the Whitman Scientist Program at the Marine Biological Laboratory.

Continue Reading

Agriculture and Food

“Stronger Social Ties, Stronger Babies: How Female Friendships Help Chimpanzee Infants Survive”

Female chimpanzees that forge strong, grooming-rich friendships with other females dramatically boost their infants’ odds of making it past the perilous first year—no kin required. Three decades of Gombe observations show that well-integrated mothers enjoy a survival rate of up to 95% for their young, regardless of male allies or sisters. The payoff may come from shared defense, reduced stress, or better access to food, hinting that such alliances laid early groundwork for humanity’s extraordinary cooperative spirit.

Avatar photo

Published

on

In a groundbreaking study published online on June 18 in iScience, researchers have found that female chimpanzees who were more socially integrated with other females before giving birth had a significantly higher chance of raising surviving offspring. This discovery sheds light on the crucial role of social connections among female chimps, particularly in the absence of close kin.

The study, led by Joseph Feldblum, assistant research professor of evolutionary anthropology at Duke University, analyzed three decades’ worth of behavioral data from 37 mothers and their 110 offspring. The researchers focused on association and grooming behavior – how often females spent time near each other or engaged in social grooming – in the year before birth.

The results showed that females who were more socially connected had a considerable better chance of raising their babies through to their first year, the period of highest infant mortality. In fact, a female with a sociality score twice the community average had a 95% chance her infant would survive the first year, while one who was halfway below average saw that chance drop to 75%. The effect persisted through age five, which is roughly the age of weaning.

Interestingly, the researchers found that having close female kin in the group – like a sister or mother – did not account for the survival benefit. Neither did having bonds with males, who could potentially offer protection. What mattered most was having social connections with other females, regardless of kinship.

“This tells us it’s not just about being born into a supportive family,” said Feldblum. “These are primarily social relationships with non-kin.”

The researchers propose several possibilities for the survival benefit, including:

* Social females receiving less harassment from other females
* More help defending food patches or protecting their young
* Offspring being less likely to be killed by another group member
* Social connections helping these females stay in better condition – maybe better fed and less stressed – through pregnancy, giving their offspring a better chance from the get-go.

Moreover, social females stayed social after their babies were born – a sign of stable relationships, not short-term alliances. “Our results don’t prove causation, but they point to the value of being surrounded by others who support you, or at least tolerate you,” said Feldblum.

This study has significant implications for understanding human evolution and cooperation. As Feldblum noted, “Human females who don’t have access to kin – for example because they moved to a new city or village – are still able to form strong bonds that can benefit them.” Studying these social dynamics in chimpanzees can help us understand how we evolved to be the social, cooperative species we are today.

Continue Reading

Trending