Connect with us

Apes

The Double-Edged Nature of Friendship in Gorillas Revealed

Friendship comes with complex pros and cons — possibly explaining why some individuals are less sociable, according to a new study of gorillas.

Avatar photo

Published

on

The intricate dynamics of friendship have long been a subject of interest among scientists, and now, a study on mountain gorillas has shed light on the complex pros and cons of these relationships. The research, conducted by the Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund and the universities of Exeter and Zurich, analyzed over 20 years of data on 164 wild mountain gorillas to examine how their social lives affected their health.

The study’s findings revealed that friendly females in small groups did not get ill often but had fewer offspring, while those in large groups got ill more frequently but had higher birth rates. In contrast, males with strong social bonds tended to get ill more often but were less likely to be injured in fights. The researchers propose that the stress of defending females and offspring may have contributed to the reduced immune function in males.

The study’s lead author, Dr Robin Morrison, highlighted the importance of considering both the benefits and costs of social relationships, stating, “Having a lot of strong social relationships is often really good – but sometimes it isn’t.” The research focused on the strength of each gorilla’s key social bonds, its integration in the group, and wider context such as group size, stability, and conflict with other groups.

The findings emphasize the forces that affect the evolution of social behavior. Dr Sam Ellis from the University of Exeter explained, “With these forces pushing in different directions, the ‘optimal’ social type will depend on an individual’s sex, age, offspring, and wider social group.” The study shows that the social environment is a strong predictor of health and lifespan in humans and other social mammals, but it is not a straightforward case of more and stronger social ties always being better.

The research was based on long-term observations of mountain gorillas in Volcanoes National Park, Rwanda. Dr Tara Stoinski, CEO and Chief Scientific Officer of the Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund, noted that this paper highlights the value of long-term studies in furthering our understanding of the evolution of sociality and how the benefits or costs of sociality can vary considerably across different environments.

The study was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation and the Dian Fossey Gorilla Fund. The research is published in the journal Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, with the title “Group traits moderate the relationship between individual social traits and fitness in gorillas.”

Animal Learning and Intelligence

Unlocking Orangutan Potential: How Zoo Life Boosts Exploration and Learning

A new study comparing wild and zoo-housed Sumatran orangutans reveals that life in a zoo significantly alters how orangutans interact with their environment. Researchers analyzed over 12,000 instances of daily exploratory object manipulation (EOM) — the active manipulation and visual inspection of objects associated with learning and problem-solving — across 51 orangutans aged 0.5 to 76 years. The findings show that orangutans living in zoos engage in more frequent, more diverse, and more complex exploration than their wild counterparts.

Avatar photo

Published

on

The lives of Sumatran orangutans living in zoos have been found to significantly alter how they interact with their environment, according to a recent study comparing wild and zoo-housed individuals. Researchers analyzed over 12,000 instances of daily exploratory object manipulation (EOM), revealing that orangutans in zoos engage in more frequent, diverse, and complex exploration than their wild counterparts.

“Our study shows that orangutans in zoos not only explore more but also explore differently,” said Isabelle Laumer, first author of the study. “What’s especially fascinating is that even when exploring the same kinds of objects, zoo-housed orangutans showed a richer repertoire of actions and were more likely to use tools or manipulate multiple objects at the same time.”

The study compared EOM behaviors in wild and zoo-housed orangutans across a wide age range. Data were collected from 33 wild individuals aged between six months and 76 years, as well as from 24 individuals in four zoos in Germany and Switzerland, with ages ranging from seven months to 49 years.

The results showed that zoo-housed orangutans explored objects more frequently than their wild counterparts, while there was no difference in exploration duration when individuals did explore. In contrast, wild orangutans primarily explored naturally occurring objects like plants, bark, and sticks, whereas zoo-housed orangutans engaged with a wider variety of enrichment items designed to encourage manipulation and cognitive engagement.

Importantly, the age at which orangutans first engaged in specific types of exploration was consistent across both settings, suggesting an innate developmental sequence. However, zoo orangutans continued to explore into adulthood, while wild individuals’ EOM declined sharply around weaning age (about 8 years old), likely due to the demands of survival in the wild.

In human infants, object exploration enables learning about physical properties such as texture and weight while stimulating cognitive and motor development – a pattern observed in many non-human animals as well. The heightened exploration may enhance cognitive flexibility and problem-solving skills in zoo-housed orangutans, as they interact with varied enrichment items and have more time and energy to devote to learning through exploration.

“These findings underscore how profoundly the environment influences animal behavior and cognitive development,” said Caroline Schuppli, senior author of the study. “And it also offers unique opportunities – by comparing wild and zoo-housed animals, we can better understand the full extent of a species’ cognitive potential.”

Continue Reading

Animals

Wild Chimpanzees’ Boozy Snack: A Taste of Evolutionary History

Wild chimpanzees have been pictured eating and sharing fruit containing alcohol.

Avatar photo

Published

on

Rewritten Article:

In a groundbreaking study, wild chimpanzees have been observed eating and sharing fruit that contains alcohol. Researchers from the University of Exeter set up motion-activated cameras in Guinea-Bissau’s Cantanhez National Park to capture footage of these fascinating creatures.

For the first time, scientists have confirmed that chimps are consuming fermented African breadfruit, which contains ethanol (alcohol). This raises intriguing questions about whether and why our closest relatives deliberately seek out alcohol.

In humans, drinking alcohol leads to a release of dopamine and endorphins, resulting in feelings of happiness and relaxation. Sharing alcohol through traditions like feasting helps form and strengthen social bonds. Could wild chimpanzees be getting similar benefits from eating fermented fruits?

The researchers used cameras to film chimps sharing fermented breadfruits on 10 separate occasions. The fruit was tested for alcohol content, with the highest level found being equivalent to 0.61% ABV (Alcohol By Volume). While this is relatively low, the scientists suggest it may be just the tip of the iceberg, as 60-85% of chimps’ diet consists of fruit, which could lead to significant consumption.

The researchers emphasize that chimps are unlikely to get “drunk,” as this would clearly not improve their survival chances. However, recent discoveries about a molecular adaptation that increased ethanol metabolism in the common ancestor of African apes suggest eating fermented fruits may have ancient origins in species including humans and chimps.

Dr Kimberley Hockings from the University of Exeter notes, “Chimps don’t share food all the time, so this behavior with fermented fruit might be important. We need to find out more about whether they deliberately seek out ethanolic fruits and how they metabolize it.” If so, it suggests the human tradition of feasting may have its origins deep in our evolutionary history.

Anna Bowland’s work was funded by the Primate Society of Great Britain, providing a fascinating glimpse into the behavior and biology of our closest relatives.

Continue Reading

Apes

A Revolutionary Robotic Gripper Made from Measuring Tape Revolutionizes Fruit and Veggie Picking

It’s a game a lot of us played as children — and maybe even later in life: unspooling measuring tape to see how far it would extend before bending. But to engineer, this game was an inspiration, suggesting that measuring tape could become a great material for a robotic gripper. The grippers would be a particularly good fit for agriculture applications, as their extremities are soft enough to grab fragile fruits and vegetables, researchers wrote. The devices are also low-cost and safe around humans.

Avatar photo

Published

on

The team of engineers at the University of California San Diego has developed a game-changing robotic gripper made from measuring tape, which could potentially revolutionize fruit and veggie picking. The device, dubbed GRIP-tape, is designed to gently grasp fragile fruits and vegetables, making it an ideal solution for agriculture applications. This low-cost, human-safe robot uses the entire length of the measuring tape as a gripping surface, allowing it to navigate through obstacles and effortlessly pick up objects with different shapes and stiffness.

The gripper’s soft yet robust design enables it to expand and contract, allowing it to reach far and wide without needing additional mechanisms. The team bound two spools of measuring tape together with adhesive to create the perfect configuration for a gripper. Each finger is controlled by four motors that can move independently, giving the robot unparalleled flexibility and precision.

The researchers had previously worked with measuring tape as part of a grant from the National Science Foundation to investigate soft materials that could bend while holding their shape. They discovered that measuring tape was an ideal material due to its springy nature, durability, and thinness, making it perfect for delicate objects.

The gripper’s unique design allows it to rotate objects or act as a conveyor belt, depositing the grasped items into containers with ease. It can pick up a wide range of objects, from small fruits like tomatoes to large ones like lemons, and even navigate through crowded farms with its flexible tape fingers.

Experiments showed that the gripper could easily lift large fruits like fresh lemons, demonstrating its potential for efficient fruit and veggie picking. Next versions of the gripper could improve on the original by adding advanced sensors and AI-driven data analysis, allowing it to operate autonomously and making it an even more valuable tool for farmers.

The work was partially funded by the National Science Foundation, and the team’s innovative design is set to transform the way we harvest crops, making it more efficient, safe, and cost-effective.

Continue Reading

Trending